The Paradox of Choice
The Paradox of Choice
“After millions of years of survival based on simple distinctions, it
may simply be that we are biologically unprepared for the number of choices we
face in the modern world” Schwartz, 2004
It’s no secret that as humans we like to
exercise our freedom of choice. Having choice is important as it helps us to
express our identity and allows us to consume what we like according to our
personal preferences. Whether it’s selecting what to wear, deciding what
television show to watch, or choosing what to do over the weekend – we strive
to be authors of our own destiny.
On the flip side, I’m sure you can recall a
time when you had too many choices, so many, in fact, that it was almost
debilitating. Perhaps you were unsure of what topping to get on your pizza? Or
maybe you were so overwhelmed by choice of ice cream flavours that you
defaulted to simply vanilla?
Known as the “choice paradox”, this fear of
making the ‘wrong choice’ or missing out on the ‘better option’ – is the
brain’s response to having too many choices.
Psychologist Barry Schwartz takes aim at a central tenet of
western societies: freedom of choice. In Schwartz's estimation, choice has made
us not freer but more paralyzed, not happier but more dissatisfied.
The Paradox
of Choice, turned traditional thinking about wealth, consumerism and modern
life on its head. Rather than increasing our sense of well-being, Schwartz says
that too many
choices in life can cause paralysis as well as increasing
levels of anxiety,
depression, and wasted time.
Schwartz
makes the point that the choice isn’t always a good thing and that at some
point too many choices is a bad as no choice at all. His theory is more
involved than that but that idea in particular is intriguing.
To illustrate,
Schwartz makes the distinction between two types of people, maximizers,
– who are not happy until they have obtained the best, and satisficers,
who tend to settle for what’s ‘good enough’. Studies show that maximizers
are less happy,
less optimistic, and have lower self-esteem than satisficers.
Maximizers are people who given a choice, will exhaustively
search all the options seeking all possible information, in order to make the
best possible choice. This behaviour generally consumes a lot of time, and
often leads to nagging doubts, perhaps where no clear winner emerged.
‘satisficers are those who settle for a choice that is good enough for them.
These people are generally happier with their choice and spend less time
choosing, leaving them to free to enjoy
other things.
How We Become Less Satisfied
In the Paradox
of Choice, Schwartz explains the steps in the process that make us less
satisfied.
1. You
imagine you could have made a better choice.
2. You
regret the decision, thinking it’s not perfect.
3. You imagine
what you didn’t choose is better than what you did choose.
4. Your
expectations escalate.
5. You
have less satisfaction with the results even with good results.
6. It’s
no longer possible for you to experience ple
Maximizing and
Satisficing
The freedom of choice has become deeply
ingrained in our social fabric. For decades, psychological researchers have
advocated the benefits of providing choice: it enhances feelings of autonomy
and freedom (Roets, Schwartz, & Guan, 2012), promotes one‘s sense of
personal control (Rotter, 1996; Taylor, 1989; Taylor & Brown, 1988), and
increases feelings of intrinsic motivation (deCharms, 1968; Deci, 1981; Deci
& Ryan, 1985). More recently, however, the decision-making literature has
undergone a shift in perspective: increased choice may actually be detrimental
and unappealing for some decision-makers. Indeed, studies conducted by Iyengar
and Lepper (1999, 2000) found that those provided with fewer options in a
decision-making task derived greater satisfaction from their decision outcomes.
Rational
choice theory is a well-established tenet in economics that rests on the
assumption that people are rational agents. Armed with complete information
about their choices, rational individuals will always choose the option that
maximizes their utility. These ―maximizers‖ approach decision-making with the
goal of achieving the best possible decision outcome. In order to accomplish
this, they are willing to engage in an exhaustive search of all possible
options, investing substantial time and effort in the decision process
(Iyengar, Wells, & Schwartz, 2006). However, behavioral economists contend
that the assumption of ―complete information‖ in decision-making is unrealistic
and that human beings often violate the principles of rational choice theory
(Kahneman & Tversky, 1979, 1984).
Rational-choice theory attempts to
explain human choice by assuming we are rational choosers, have well
ordered preferences, and have information on costs and benefits.
It also assumes we compare options on the basis of preference, value
or utility. Additionally, the theory says that rational choosers
should always be able to express their preferences.
A rational decision maker,
therefore, will look at all the options, choose the one that brings the
most utility, and understand why the choice was made. But in economic
theory, there hasn’t been enough study understanding from where our
preferences come. Preferences are complicated because it includes human
biases as well as culture.
More
than half a century ago, Herbert Simon (1955, 1956) introduced a theory that
addressed the limitations of human cognition as well as environmental
complexities in the decision-making process. He argued that the goal of utility
maximization, as stipulated by rational choice theory, is nearly impossible to
achieve in real life. Rather than maximize, people often ―satisfice‖ when
making decisions.
Satisficers
have an internal threshold of acceptability against which they evaluate
options, and will choose a decision outcome when it crosses this threshold.
Therefore, satisficers are content to settle for a ―good enough‖ option—not
necessarily the very best outcome in all respects.
More
recently, Schwartz and his colleagues (2002) characterized this tendency to
maximize or satisfice into an individual, psychological trait. They developed a
13-item Maximization Scale in order to assess an individual‘s tendency to seek
optimality in decision-making, testing facets related to alternative search,
decision difficulty, and high standards (Roets et al., 2012). Compared to
satisfiers, maximizing individuals are more likely to experience lower levels
of happiness, life satisfaction, optimism, and self-esteem.
In
addition, maximizing tendencies were shown to have significant, positive
correlations with regret, perfectionism, and depression (Schwartz et al.,
2002). Despite their high-effort decision-making process, maximizers are less
satisfied with their final decision outcomes than satisfiers.
The
negative affect experienced by maximizers can be attributed to the presence and
proliferation of choices in the decision-making process. In order to determine
their optimal decision outcome, maximizers feel compelled to examine each and
every alternative available, which is often infeasible in reality due to the
limitations in human cognition (Roets et al., 2012). For maximizers, the excess
of options becomes problematic for several reasons. First, it grows increasingly
difficult to collect and process the information necessary to construct an
informed, complete set of options.
Second,
choice proliferation makes it more difficult to correctly identify the ―best‖
outcome on an objective basis. This forces maximizers to rely on external
sources of information to evaluate their options (Iyengar et al., 2006). In
fact, maximizing individuals are more likely to engage in upward social
comparisons in order to gauge the optimality of their decisions. This
encourages counterproductive thinking about ―what might have been‖, which
perpetuates feelings of regret (Schwartz et al., 2002).
Third,
as the number of available choices increase, a maximizer‘s standards of an
acceptable outcome inflates correspondingly. Given the practical constraints on
conducting an exhaustive search, a maximizer‘s high expectations inevitably
lead to disappointment and dissatisfaction with his final decision.
Fourth,
maximizers may be more likely to attribute failures or poor decisions to personal
shortcomings rather than situational limitations and environmental
complexities. This depressogenic way of thinking causes maximizing individuals
to have lower self-esteem than their satisficing counterparts (Schwartz et al.,
2002; Polman, 2010).
Finally,
more choices imply a higher probability that an individual will make a
non-optimal decision. This can indirectly undermine the satisfaction a
maximizer derives from his actual choice (Schwartz et al., 2002; Polman, 2010).
As Roets et al. argue, ―there is always the possibility that there is a better
option ‗out there‘, and failing to find it means a failure to optimize personal
satisfaction‖ (Roets et al., 2012).
Most
recently, Sparks and her colleagues (2012) found that maximizers are more
reluctant to commit to their choices. Their reticence to commit robs them of
critical, post-decision psychological processes, such as dissonance reduction
and rationalization (Sparks, Ehrlinger, & Eibach, 2012). Ultimately, this
leaves maximizing individuals feeling less satisfied with their decision
outcomes. The presence of choice contributes to the heightened feelings of
regret, unrealistic expectations, and high opportunity costs suffered by
maximizers.
Satisficers,
on the other hand, undergo a fundamentally different, decision-making process.
With modest standards for what constitutes an acceptable decision outcome, a
satisficing individual does not require a complete information set when making
his decisions. Several options may fall within a satisficer‘s threshold for
acceptability, providing greater flexibility and latitude in achieving a
desired decision outcome. As soon as he encounters a ―good enough‖ option, the
satisficer can easily ignore the addition of new choices to the decision
domain. Therefore, a satisficer is less likely to experience regret even if a
better option presents itself after a decision has already been made (Schwartz
et al., 2002).
Given
all this, are maximizers rewarded for their troubles by achieving better
decision outcomes? Does their high-effort decision-making process result in
better decision quality? Iyengar et al. (2006) found that recent college
graduates with high maximizing tendencies accepted jobs that paid 20% higher
starting salaries than their satisficing peers. Despite higher salaries,
however, these maximizing students were less satisfied with the jobs they
obtained. They also experienced more negative affect both during and after the
job search process (Iyengar et al., 2006). When compared to satisficers, it
appears that maximizing individuals generally achieve better outcomes
objectively, but perceive them to be worse subjectively for the reasons
discussed above (Schwartz et al., 2002; Iyengar et al., 2006).
However,
this view has been openly debated in the decision-making literature. Parker,
Bruine de Bruin, and Fischhoff (2007) found that self-reported maximizers are
more likely to use maladaptive decision-making styles. A tendency to maximize
results in less behavioral coping, greater dependence on others for information,
increased interpersonal comparisons, avoidance of decision-making in order to
search for more information, and more acute feelings of regret (Parker et al.,
2007). These findings were consistent with those of Bruine de Bruin et al.
(2007): individuals who scored highly on Schwartz‘s Maximization Scale were
poorer decision makers when measured by the Decision-Making Competence survey
and self-reported Decision Outcomes Inventory (Bruine de Bruin, Parker, &
Fischhoff, 2007).
Contrary
to intuition, maximizers are also more likely to engage in spontaneous
decision-making (Parker et al., 2007). Overall, satisficing individuals
achieved better decision outcomes (cf. Iyengar et al., 2006).
In Barry Schwartz’s famous Ted Talk on choice, he suggests that offering too much choice may
result in 3 negative side effects:
1. Analysis Paralysis: When there are too many
options, customers will become paralysed and instead purchase nothing.
Whirlpool recently tested this via an email marketing campaign. By
significantly reducing the number of products advertised, Whirlpool saw
engagement with their advertisement rise by a whopping 42%.
2. Buyers’ remorse: After browsing for hours
and then finally settling on a product, many customers will experience a sudden
pang of guilt. This is a reflection of them second-guessing whether they’ve
made the right choice.
3. Decision fatigue: Put simply, we each have
the energy to make a finite number of decisions each day. Once we’ve reached
that limit, our ability to make smart decisions (or any decision at all) is
severely depleted.
Barry Schwarz explores this
shortcoming as well as gives us evidence-based advice on how we can
make wiser decisions in his book The Paradox of Choice:
Why More Is Less
. He lays out a compelling case for becoming a Satisfier (one
who makes a decision when their criteria is met) over a Maximizers (one who
must make the best choice). With the overload of options we have in
modern times, we can end up more spending an enormous amount of time
researching options and making the subsequent choices. Schwartz makes a
compelling argument on why Satisficers tend to be happier than Maximizers.
Maximizers must spend loads of time and energy to reach a decision, and in the
end, they are often anxious about those choices and much more likely to
ruminate. As a Satisfier, you can still have high standards, but you make the
choice when your criteria is met.
Most of us in business have heard the
advice by Schwarz and many that we should ignore sunk costs, which are
historical and no longer have any bearing on the go-forward
situation. In the case of opportunity costs, alternatives are still
relevant, but Schwarz advises they should be considered because it’s very
difficult to adequately weigh whether our front-running choice is
indeed good without knowing the alternatives. He cautions we should limit
the set of possibilities considered among opportunity costs to reduce
negative psychological impact.
Examples of other advisable strategies
from The Paradox of Choice:
Why More Is Less
include 1) choosing when to choose (narrow
down choices by constraining when we choose, 2) deciding not
to choose at all (when the decision is not important), 3) increasing
the frequency of making a choice when you find an option that is good
enough (“satisficing”), and 4) having second-order decisions (preset
choices in certain situations).
Eg- The 'paradox of choice' could explain why
you're still single - here's what it means
·
Dating apps mean we are
given nearly endless choices of who we can date.
·
While this should make
connections easier, it also makes us more picky.
·
This is because of the
"paradox of choice" that makes us believe the grass is always greener
on the other side.
·
By always looking for
something better, you might miss the opportunity right in front of your eyes.
But if you're spending this
Valentine's Day crying over the fact nobody wants to be in a relationship with
you, there's a psychological reason that might help explain why.
It's called
"the paradox of choice," and it essentially means that while we
consider variety as a good thing, at the same time, it makes our decisions more
challenging.
For example, you may have met
someone on on Tinder, and the first date went really well. You probably want to
see them again, but you can't help noticing their tiny flaws. You know your
online profile is sitting there on your phone, and you just can't shake the
feeling there could be someone else on the dating app that would be an even
better fit for you.
"Maximizers treat
relationships like clothing," he writes. "I expect to try a lot on
before finding the perfect fit. For a maximizer, somewhere out there is the
perfect lover, the perfect friends. Even though there is nothing wrong with the
current relationship, who knows what's possible if you keep your eyes open."
The opposite of maximisers are
"satisficers," who have the ability to know a good thing when they
see it, without obsessing over "what ifs."
It's not the same as settling for
a bad option, because satisficing also means having high standards. But it does
also mean ignoring the temptation of finding out if the grass really is greener
on the other side.
In theory, it
makes sense. If you're always holding out for something better, chances are
you'll end up with nothing. That, or you'll realise you left all your good
options in the cold, and you'll end up with someone who's wrong for you. By
that logic, satisficers are more likely to end up happy.
In a blog post about this for Psychology Today, Jen Kim
writes about how in modern dating life, we no longer have the feeling of
scarcity, as there are always so many options at our fingertips. This doesn't
just make us picky, but arguably unreasonably so.
"How quickly have we thumbed
left simply because the face peering back at us had an eyebrow hair out of
place or because the guy seemed short even though you could only see his
head?" she writes. "How many amazing potential mates have we missed
out on because we were convinced the next profile would be better?"
In the end, attraction is about
more than just a photo. It's more than just an instant spark on a first date,
or a Valentine's Day card.
Ultimately,
while dating apps bring us closer to people we might not otherwise have
met, the issues they cause paradoxically
make it even more difficult to make a connection.
To avoid falling in the
maximising trap, if you think you've met someone and it could be something
good, try and give it a fair chance. Otherwise you might be holding out for a
fairytale that could never happen.
In Barry Schwartz's seminal book, "The Paradox of Choice,"
we learn that while choice is a vital part of autonomy and fundamental to our
well-being, too much choice has a cost, and our obsession with it contributes
to bad decisions, anxiety, stress, dissatisfaction, paralysis, and even
depression.
From choosing the best tacos to
finding the ideal mate, today's perils of choice come from every direction.
Almost every part of daily life requires us to choose, compelling many to waste
a ton of time and energy on research for fear that the wrong decision might get
made.
Here are some of the ways too many options
mess with us, according to psychological research:
Whenever you choose one thing, you're also not choosing other things that could be just as good
Whenever you choose one thing, you're also not choosing other things that could be just as good
One of the problems with having too
many options before us is that each one comes with its trade-offs, Schwartz says, and trade-offs have psychological
consequences.
When
there are lots of alternatives to consider, it's easy to imagine the attractive
features of the alternatives you reject.
"The
necessity of making trade-offs alters how we feel about the decisions we face;
more important, it affects the level of satisfaction we experience from the
decisions we ultimately make," he writes.
Greater choice makes us fantasize about a better option out there
When there are multiple
alternatives, Schwartz says, it's also easy for
us to imagine alternatives that don't actually exist and are a combination of
the attractive features of the alternatives that do exist. We've essentially
created greater expectations than we could possibly meet.
"To
the extent that we engage our imaginations in this way, we will be even less
satisfied with the alternative we end up choosing," he writes. Greater
variety in this way actually makes us feel worse off.
More options lead to greater regret
Even if you've made a good
decision, Schwartz says, when your choice isn't perfect, knowing
there were alternatives out there makes it easy to imagine you could have made
a better choice.
This
leads you to regret the decision you made, which leads to dissatisfaction, even
if it was a good decision.
Too many options can lead us to make bad decisions
The emotional cost of potential
trade-offs also interferes with the quality of decisions we make, Schwartz says. When we feel bad about choosing, we
begin to lose focus and instead of examining all aspects of a decision, we home
in on a couple of aspects, some that might not be that important.
Our negative emotions associated with
having to choose can also distract us from the decision itself, Schwartz says, which impairs our decision-making
abilities.
People don't like feeling bad about their decisions, which can lead to paralysis of action
People tend to resist making
decisions when there are so many trade-offs, which can lead to postponing or
avoiding making the decision, Schwartz says.
This may not be so serious when
you're choosing which new smartphone to buy or what to eat for lunch, but it
can have detrimental affects on your future, like when choosing which
retirement savings plan to settle on.
The conflict that comes from too many choices can also cause paralysis
Scwartz points to
numerous studies that found when there are two options with a clear winner,
most people made a decision — but when people are presented with options
involving trade-offs that create conflict, all choices begin to look
unappealing, and people are less likely to make a decision.
An abundance of choices leaves less time to make the right decision
"Nobody has the time or
cognitive resources to be completely thorough and accurate with every decision,
and as more decisions are required and more options are available, the
challenge of doing the decision making correctly becomes ever more difficult to
meet," Schwartz writes.
And choosing eats up our time, a precious commodity we often have so little of
Schwartz says
preparing for, making, reevaluating, and perhaps regretting the vastly greater
number of choices we have today eats up one of our most valuable resources:
time.
"Time
spent dealing with choice is time taken away from being a good friend, a good
spouse, a good parent, and a good congregant," he says.
People who always want the best option are often more depressed than people who settle
In Schwartz's study
of people who always look for the best option, whom he calls maximizers,
compared with people who are OK settling for something that's good enough, whom
he calls satisficers, he found maximizers experienced less satisfaction with
life, were less happy, were less optimistic, and were more depressed than
satisficers.
While Schwartz notes
that the study finding does not mean being a maximizer causes unhappiness, he
personally believes that being a maximizer plays a causal role in people's
unhappiness.
Choosing between too many options can cause decision fatigue, which can lead to worse decisions down the road
Some psychologists
believe that making a number of small decisions like what to
wear and what to eat wears out your brain and saps the mental power that you
could be using for more pressing matters, which is often referred to as decision
fatigue.
They
posit that your pool of decision-making energy is limited, and eventually, as
your willpower depletes with each new decision you make, you're more likely to
either act impulsively or do nothing.
It's
why the likes of President Barack Obama and Steve Jobs limited their clothing
options — making too many trivial decisions would waste their ability to make
other, more pressing decisions down the road.
As Obama explained during
a 2012 interview with Vanity Fair when he revealed why he only
wears gray or blue suits:
I'm
trying to pare down decisions. I don't want to make decisions about what I’m
eating or wearing. Because I have too many other decisions to make.
These days, the fear of missing out extends well beyond what we should buy or eat
"It seems to me that the most
striking trend is the appearance of social media,"Schwartz told Pacific Mag last
year. "My suspicion is that it and dating sites have created
just the thing I talk about in connection with consumer goods: Nobody's good
enough and you're always worried you're missing out."
"Nobody makes plans because
something better might turn up, and the result is that nobody ever does
anything," Schwartz says.
We have all been
there: standing in aisle five of the supermarket trying to decide which jar of
mustard to buy. Do we go organic, or for the brand with whole mustard seeds? Or
do we simply pick the one in the brightest yellow bottle?
In a fascinating talk
at TEDxStanford, “Sometimes
it’s good to give up the driver’s seat,” marketing professor Baba
Shiv reveals that discomfort over making choices extends into medical
decisions. Five years ago, Shiv’s wife was diagnosed with breast cancer.
“The most harrowing
and agonizing part of the whole experience was that we were making decision
after decision,” Shiv shares in his
talk. “The wisdom of the ages is that when it comes to decisions of
importance, it’s best to be in charge. But are there contexts where we’re far
better off taking the passenger seat and having someone else drive?”
Shiv decided to test
the theory on undergraduate students about to solve word puzzles. While one set
of students was asked to chose between two teas — caffeinated or relaxing
chamomile — the other group was told by the researchers which of the teas to
drink. In the end, the students assigned a tea solved more puzzles than those
who were given a choice. Shiv hypothesized that this is because making the
choice allows a person to have doubt about their decision when faced with the
prospect of immediate feedback.
Shiv’s thoughts on
choice are counterintuitive. But his work is part of a growing body of research
on choice. Below, more studies — many from TED speakers — which suggest that
having a variety of options isn’t always what we need.
In a jam
TED speaker Sheena Iyengar, a professor at Columbia University, performed a classic experiment in the realm of choice studies in 1995. In the study — which she describes in her TEDTalk “How to Make Choosing Easier” — Iyengar presented shoppers in a gourmet market with a display of jams. At times, the display showed 24 varieties. At others, it included only six. Iyengar found that, yes, 60 percent of customers found themselves pulled to the large display while only 40 percent stopped at the small one. But with 24 possible options, consumers questioned themselves and only 3% made a jam purchase. At the small display, nearly a third of consumers who stopped by bought a jar of jam.
TED speaker Sheena Iyengar, a professor at Columbia University, performed a classic experiment in the realm of choice studies in 1995. In the study — which she describes in her TEDTalk “How to Make Choosing Easier” — Iyengar presented shoppers in a gourmet market with a display of jams. At times, the display showed 24 varieties. At others, it included only six. Iyengar found that, yes, 60 percent of customers found themselves pulled to the large display while only 40 percent stopped at the small one. But with 24 possible options, consumers questioned themselves and only 3% made a jam purchase. At the small display, nearly a third of consumers who stopped by bought a jar of jam.
The pasta
problem
Malcolm Gladwell also thinks extensively about choice, and in his riveting TEDTalk “Malcolm Gladwell on spaghetti sauce,” he describes a visionary who anticipated Iyengar’s findings more than a decade before they were made. Howard Moskowitz, a psychophysicist turned market researcher, was asked by Prego spaghetti sauce in the early ‘80s to help them revise their product line. And thus Moskowitz headed out on the road with 45 pasta sauces, asking thousands of Americans to rate each one. But, using knowledge gleaned from working for brands like Pepsi and Vlassic Pickles, Moskowitz recommended that — rather than offering.
Malcolm Gladwell also thinks extensively about choice, and in his riveting TEDTalk “Malcolm Gladwell on spaghetti sauce,” he describes a visionary who anticipated Iyengar’s findings more than a decade before they were made. Howard Moskowitz, a psychophysicist turned market researcher, was asked by Prego spaghetti sauce in the early ‘80s to help them revise their product line. And thus Moskowitz headed out on the road with 45 pasta sauces, asking thousands of Americans to rate each one. But, using knowledge gleaned from working for brands like Pepsi and Vlassic Pickles, Moskowitz recommended that — rather than offering.
References
o
https://www.businessinsider.in/11-ways-having-too-many-options-is-screwing-us-up/articleshow/55710044.cms
o
Starry Peng (2013), Maximizing and Satisficing
in Decision-Making Dyads, University of Pennsylvania
Comments
Post a Comment